Bibliography for Animal Learning

Whole book (full text) Whole book (pdf —2.1MB)
Home page Index to teaching materials
Lectures notes for further lectures on Pavlovian conditioning in humans and other species
Chapter on classical conditioning in an earlier bookand in a much earlier book.

 

 

 

 

Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., and Teasdale, J. D. (1978). Learned helplessness in humans: critique and reformulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49—74.

Adams, C. D. (1982). Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34B, 77—98.

Adams, C. D. and Dickinson, A. (l981a). Instrumental responding following reinforcer devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33B, 109—21.

Adams, C. D. and Dickinson, A. (l981b). Actions and habits. In Spear, N. E. and Miller, R. R. (eds). Information Processing in Animals. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hilisdale, New Jersey, 143—65.

Adams, R. M. and Walker, S. F. (1972). Stimulus control of counting-like behaviour in rats. Psychonomic Science, 29, 167—9.

Ader, R. (1976). Conditioned adrenocortical steroid elevations in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 90, 1156—63.

Alloy, L. B. amd Tabachnik, N. (1984). Assessment of covariation by humans and animals: the joint influence of prior expectations and current situational information. Psychological Review, 91, 1 12—49.

Alvarez-BuyIla, R. and Alvarez-Buylla, E. R. (1975). Hypoglycemic conditioned reflex in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 155—60.

Amsel, A. (1962) Frustrative nonreward in partial reinforcement and discrimination learning. Psychological Review, 69, 306—28.

Anderson, J. A. and Mozer, M. C. (1981). Categorization and selective neurons. In Hinton, G. E. and Anderson, J. A. (eds). Parallel Models of Associative Memory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 2 13-36.

Andrews, A. E. and Braveman, N. S. (1975) The combined effects of dosage level and interstimulus interval on the formation of one-trial poison-based aversions in rats. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 3, 287—9.

Anger, D. (1956). The dependence of interresponse times upon the relative reinforcement of different interresponse times. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, 145—61.

Archer, T., Nilsson, L. G. and Carter, N. (1979) Role of exteroceptive background context in taste-aversion conditioning and extinction. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 7, 17—22.

Aristotle (c. 350 BC/1927—52). The Works of Aristotle. Clarendon Press: Oxford.

Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and submission to group pressure: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70, no. 416.

Asratyan, E. A. (1953). I. P. Pavlov: His Life and Work. Foreign Languages Publishing House: Moscow.

Asratyan, E. A. (1965). Conditioned Reflex and Compensatory Mechanisms. Pergamon: Oxford.

Atkinson, R. C. and Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: a proposed system and its control processes. In Spence, K. W. and Spence, J. T. (eds). The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Vol. 2. Academic Press: New York, 89—195.

Azrin, N. A. and Holz, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In Honig, W. K. (ed.), Operant Behaviour. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, 380—447.

Babkin, B. P. (1949). Pavlov, A Biography. University of Chicago Press. Badia, P., Harsh, J. and Abbott, B. (1979). Choosing between predictable and undpredictable shock conditions: data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 1107—31.

Baernstein, H. D. and Hull, C. L. (1931). A mechanical model of the conditioned reflex. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 99—106.

Baker, A. G. and Mackintosh, N. J. (1977). Excitatory and inhibitory conditioning following uncorrelated presentations of CS and UCS. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 5, 3 15- 19.

Baker, M. C. and Cunningham, M. A. (1985). The biology of bird- song dialects. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 8, 85—134.

Balda, R. P. and Turek, R. J. (1984). The cache-recovery system as an example of memory capabilities in Clark’s nutcracker. In Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, 513—32.

Ballard, D. H., Hinton, G. E. and Sejnowski, T. J. (1984). Parallel visual computation. Nature, 306, 21—6.

Barber, P. J. and Legge, D. (1986). Information and Human Performance. Methuen: London.

Barlow, H. B. (1972). Single units and sensation: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology?. Perception, 1, 371—94.

Bateson, P. P. G. (1966). The characteristics and content of imprinting. Biological Review, 41, 177—220.

Bateson. P. P. G. (1973). Preferences for familiarity and novelty: a model for the simultaneous development of both. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 41, 249—59.

Bateson, P. P. G. (1979). How do sensitive periods arise and what are they for? Animal Behaviour, 27, 470—86.

Bateson, P. P. G. and Chantry, D. F. (1972). Retardation of discrimination learning in monkeys and chicks previously exposed to both stimuli. Nature, 237, 173—4.

Baum, W. M. (1974). On two types of deviation from the matching law: bias and undermatching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 22, 231—42.

Baum, W. M. (1981). Optimization and the matching law as accounts of instrumental behaviour. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 20, 387—403.

Becker,J. T. and Olton, D. 5. (1982). Cognitive mapping and hippocampal function. Neuropsychologia, 19, 733—41.

Beggs, A. L., Steinmetz, J. E., Romano, A. G. and Patterson, M. M. (1983). Extinction and retention of a classically conditioned flexor nerve response in acute spinal cat. Behavioural Neuroscience, 97, 530—40.

Bendig, A. W. (1952). Latent learning in a water maze. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 143—7.

Benninger, R. J., Bellisle, F. amd Milner, P. M. (1977). Schedule control of behaviour reinforced by electrical stimulation of the brain. Science, 196, 547—9.

Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, Arousal, and Curiosity. McGraw-Hill: New York.

Bernstein, J. 5. (1961). The utilization of visual cues in dimension-abstracted oddity by primates. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 54, 243—7.

Bessemer, D. W. and Stollnitz, F. (1971). Retention of discriminations and an analysis of learning set. In Schrier, A. M. and Stollnitz, F. (eds). Behaviour of Nonhuman Primates: Vol. IV. Academic Press: New York, 1—58.

Best, P. J., Best, M. R. and Mickley, G. A. (1973). Conditioned aversion to distinct environmental stimuli resulting from gastro intestinal distress. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 85, 250—7.

Bindra, D. (1968). Neuropsychological interpretation of the effects of drive and incentive-motivation on general activity and instrumental behaviour. Psychological Review, 75,. 1—22.

Bindra, D. (1976). A Theory of Intelligent Behaviour. Wiley: London.

Black, A. H. (1959). Heart rate changes during avoidance learning ii dogs. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 13, 229—42.

Blodgett, H. C. (1929). The effect of the introduction of reward upon the maze performances of rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 113—34.

Blodgett, H. C. and McCutchan, K. (1948). Relative strength of place and response learning in the T-maze. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 41, 17—24.

Bloomfield, T. M. (1966). Two types of behavioural contrast in discrimination learning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 9, 155—61.

Blough, D, S. (1972). Recognition by the pigeon of stimuli varying in two dimensions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 18, 345—67.

Blough, D. S. (1977). Visual search in the pigeon: hunt and peck method. Science, 196, 1013—4.

Blough, D. S. (1979). Effect of the number and form of stimuli on visual search in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 5, 211—23.

Blough, D. S. (1982). Pigeon perception of letters of the alphabet. Science, 218, 397—8.

Blough, D. 5. (1984). Form recognition in pigeons. In Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, 277-89.

Blough, D. S. (1985). Discrimination of letters and random dot patterns by pigeons and humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 11, 26 1—80.

Boakes, R. A. (1977) Performance on learning to associate a stimulus with positive reinforcement. In Davis, H. and Hurwitz, H. M. B. (eds). Operant-Pavlovian Interactions. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 67—97.

Boakes, R. A. (1984). From Darwin to Behaviourism. Cambridge University Press.

Boakes, R. A., PoIi, M., Lockwood, M.J. and Goodall, G. (1978). A study of misbehaviour: token reinforcement in the rat. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 29, 115—34.

Boden, M. A. (1979). Piaget. Fontana: London.

Boe, E. E. and Church, R. M. (1967). Permanent effects of punishment during extinction. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 486—92.

Boice, R. (1984). Packrats (Neotoma albigula and N. micropus) compared in an operant anologue of foraging behaviours. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 98, 115—18.

Bolles, R. C. (1970). Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. Psychological Review, 77, 32—48.

Bolles, R. C. (1978). The role of stimulus learning in defensive behaviour. In Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H. and Honig, W. K. (eds). Cognitive Processes in Animal Behaviour. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NewJersey, 89—108.

Bond, A. B. (1983). Visual search and selection of natural stimuli in the pigeon: the attention threshold hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 9, 292—305.

Booth, D. A. and Simpson, P. C. (1971). Food preferences acquired by association with variations in amino acid nutrition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 135—45.

Booth, D. A., Lovett, D. and McSherry, G. M. (1972). Post- ingestive modulation of the sweetness preference gradient in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 78, 485—512.

Booth, D. A., Lee, M. and McAleavey, C. (1976). Acquired sensory control of satiation in man. British Journal of Psychology, 67, 137-47.

Boring, E. G. (1950). A History of Experimental Psychology. Appleton-CenturyCrofts: New York.

Born, D. G., Snow, M. E. and Herbert, E. W. (1969). Conditional discrimination learning in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 12, 119—25.

Brady, J. V., Porter, R. W., Condrad, D. G. amd Mason, J. W. (1958). Avoidance behaviour and the development of gastroduodenal ulcers. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 1, 69—72.

Breland, K. and Breland, M. (1961). The misbehaviour of organizms. American Psychologist, 16, 681—4.

British Psychological Society, Scientific Affairs Board (1979). Report of the Working Party on Animal Experimentation. Bulletin of the British Psychological Society, 32, 44—52.

Broadbent, D. E. (1958). Perception and Communication. Pergamon: Oxford. Broadbent, D. E. (1984). The Maltese cross: a new simplistic model for memory. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 7, 55—94.

Brogden, W. J. (1939). Sensory pre-conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 25, 323—32.

Brown J. S. (1942). Factors determining conflict reactions in difficult discriminations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 272—92.

Brown, J. S. (1969). Factors affecting self-punitive locomotor behaviour. In Campbell, B. A. and Church, R. M. (eds). Punishment and Aversive Behaviour. Appleton-Century- Crofts: New York, 467—5 14.

Brown, P. L. and Jenkins, H. M. (1968), Autoshaping of the pigeon’s key peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 11, 1—8.

Browne, M. P. (1976). The role of primary reinforcement and overt movements in auto-shaping in the pigeon. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 4, 287—92.

Catania, A. C. (1963). Concurrent performances: reinforcement interaction and response independence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 6, 253—63.

Catania, A. C. and Reynolds, G. S. (1970). A quantitative analysis of the responding maintained by interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 11, 327—83.

Cerella, J. (1979). Visual classes and natural categories in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 68—77.

Cerella, J. (1982). Mechanisms of concept formation in the pigeon in Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A. and Mansfield, R. J. W. (eds). Analysis of Visual Behaviour. MIT Press: London, 241—60.

Channell, S. and Hall, G. (1981). Facilitation and retardation of discrimination learning after exposure to the stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 7, 437—46.

Charnov, E. L. (1976). Optimal foraging: the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology, 9, 129-36.

Chen, J. S. and Amsel, A. (1980). Recall (versus recognition) immunization against aversive taste anticipations based on illness. Science, 209, 851—3.

Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. Mouton: The Hague.

Chomsky, N. (1976). Reflections on Language. Fontana: London.

Chomsky, N. (1980). Rules and Representations. Columbia University Press: New York.

Chopin, S. F. and Beurger, A. H. (1975). Graded acquisition of an instrumental avoidance response by the spinal rat. Physiology and Behaviour, 15, 155—8.

Clark, H. H. and Clark, E. V. (1977). Psychology and Language. Harcourt Brace: New York.

Coburn, C. A. (1914). The behaviour of the crow Corvus Americanus Aud. Journal of Animal Behaviour, 4, 185—201.

Cohen, N. J. and Squire, L. R. (1980). Preserved learning and retention of pattern analyzing in amnesia: dissociation of knowing how and knowing that. Science, 210, 207—10.

Collier, G. H. and Royce-Collier, C. K. (1981). A comparative analysis of optimal foraging behaviour: laboratory simulations. In Kamil, A. C. and Sargent, T. D. (eds). Foraging Behaviour. Garland Books: New York.

Collier, G., Hirsch, E. and Hamlin, P. H. (1972). The ecological determinants of reinforcement in the rat. Physiology and Behaviour, 9, 705— 16.

Cook, R. G. (1980). Retroactive interference in pigeon short- term memory by a reduction in ambient illumination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 6, 326—38.

Cook, R. G., Brown, M. F. and Riley, D. A. (1985). Flexible memory processing by rats: use of prospective and retrospective information in the radial maze. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 11, 453—68.

Coover, B. D., Sutton, B. R. and Heybach, J. P. (1977). Conditioning decreases in plasma cortico-sterone levels in rats by pairing stimuli with daily feedings. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91, 716—26.

Cormier, S. M. (1986). Basic Processes of Learning, Cognition and Motivation: A Match-Mismatch Theory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London.

Cornell, E. H. (1974). Infants’ discrimination of photographs of faces following redundant presentations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 98—106.

Couvillon, P. A. and Bitterman, M. E. (1982). Compound conditioning in honey bees. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 96, 192—9.

Couyillon, P. A. and Bitterman, M. E. (1984). The over learning-extinction effect and successive negative contrast in honey bees. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 98, 100—9.

Cowan, P. E. (1976). The new object reaction of Rattus rattus L.: the relative importance of various cues. Behavioural Biology, 16, 31—44.

Crespi, L. P. (1942). Quantitative variation in incentive and performance in the white rat. American Journal of Psychology, 55, 467—517.

Creutzfeldt, 0. D. (1979). Neurophysiological mechanisms and consciousness. In Brain and Mind, Ciba Foundation Symposium 69. Excertpa Medica: Amsterdam, 217—33.

Croze, H. J. (1970). Searching images in carrion crows. Zeitschrzft fόr Tierpsychologie, 5, 1—85.

Cunningham, A. (1921). A gorilla’s life in civilization. Zoological Society Bulletin, New York, 24, 118—24.

D’Amato, M. R. (1973). Delayed matching and short-term memory in monkeys. In Bower, G. H. (ed.) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Vol. 7. Academic Press: New York, 227—69.

D’Amato, M. R. and Cox, J. K. (1976). Delay of consequences and short-term memory in monkeys. In Medin, D. L., Roberts, D. A. and Davis, R. T. (eds). Processes in Animal Memory. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 49—78.

D’Amato. M. R. and Worsham, R. W. (1974). Retrieval cues and short-term memory in capuchin monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 274—82.

Darwin, C. (1872/1965). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Chicago University Press.

Dashiell, J. F. (1930). Direction orientation in maze running by the white rat. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 7, 1—72.

Davenport, R. K., Rogers, C. M. and Russell, I. 5. (1973). Cross modal perception in apes. Neuropsychologia, 11, 21—8.

Davenport, R. K., Rogers, C. M. and Russell, I. S. (1975). Cross modal perception in apes: altered visual cues and delay. Neuropsychologia, 13 229—35.

Davey, G. (1983). An associative view of human classical conditioning. In Davey, G. C. L. (ed.). Animal Models of Human Behaviour. Wiley: Chichester, 95-1 14.

Davey, G. (ed.). (1986). Conditioning in Humans. Wiley: Chichester. Davis, H. (1977). Response characteristics and control during lever press escape. In Davis, H. and Hurwitz, H.M.B. (eds). Operant-Pavlovian Interactions. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 233—66.

Davis, M. (1972). Differential retention of sensitization and habituation of the startle response in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 78, 210—67.

Davis, M. and Wagner, A. R. (1969). Habituation of startle response under incremental sequence of stimulus intensities. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 67, 486—92.

Davis, P. (1976). Conditioning after-images: a procedure minimizes the extinction effect of normal test trials. British Journal of Psychology 67, 18 1—9.

Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press: London. Dawkins, R. (1982). The Extended Phenotype. Freeman: Oxford. Day, R. H. (1972). Visual spatial illusions: a general explanation. Science, 175, 1335—40.

Deese, J. (1951). Extinction of a discrimination without performance of the choice response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 44, 362—6.

Deets, A. C., Harlow, H. F. and Blomquist, A. J. (1970). Effects of inter-trial interval and Trial 1 reward during acquisition of an object-discrimination learning set in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 73, 50 1—5.

DeLong, R. E. and Wasserman, E. A. (1981). Effects of different reinforcement expectancies on successive matching-to- sample performance in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 7, 394—412.

Dember, W. N. and Warm, J. S. (1979). Psychology of Perception, 2nd edn. Holt, Rinehart & Winston: London.

Dethier, V. G. (1963). The Physiology of Insect Senses. Methuen: London. Deutsch,J. A. (1960). The Structural Basis of Behaviour. Cambridge University Press.

Deutsch, J. A. and Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: some theoretical considerations. Psychological Review, 70, 80—90.

Deutsch, J. and Howarth C. I. (1963). Some tests of a theory of intracranial self-stimulation, Psychological Review, 70, 444—60.

De Villiers, P. A. and Herrnstein, R. J. (1976). Toward a law of response strength. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 1131—53.

Dews, P. B. (1962). The effect of multiple Sdelta periods on responding on a fixed-interval schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 5, 369—74.

Dews, P. B. (1970). The theory of fixed-interval responding. In Schoenfeld, W. N. (ed.). The Theory of Reinforcement Schedules. Appleton-CenturyCrofts: New York, 43—61.

Dickinson, A. (1980), Contemporary Animal Learning Theory. Cambridge University Press.

Dickinson, A. (1985). Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B. 308. 67—78.

Dickinson, A. (1986). Re-examination of the role of the instrumental contingency in the sodium-appetite irrelevant incentive effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38B, 16 1—72.

Dickinson, A., Nicholas, D. J. and Adams, C. D. (1983). The effect of the instrumental training contingency on susceptibility to reinforcer devaluation. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35B, 35—5 1.

Dinsmoor, J. A. and Hughes, H. L. (1956). Training rats to press a bar to turn off shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 49, 235—8.

Dinsmoor, J. A., Matsuoka, Y. and Winograd, E. (1958). Barholding as a preparatory response in escape-from-shock training. Journal of Comparative and Physiology, 51, 637—9.

Distenhoff, J. F., Haggerty, R. and Corning, V. C. (1971). An analysis of leg position learning in the cockroach yoked control. Physiology and Behaviour, 7, 359—62.

Domjan, M. and Galef, B. G. Jr. (1983). Biological constraints on instrumental and classical conditioning: retrospect and prospect. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 11, 151—6l.

Domjan, M. and Wilson, N. E. (1972). Specificity of cue to consequence in aversion learning in the rat. Psychonomic Science, 26, 143—5.

Doty, B. A., Jones, C. N., and Doty, L. A. (1967). Learning- set formation by mink, ferrets, skunks and cats. Science, 155, 1579—80.

Drent, R. H. (1982). Risk-benefit assessment in animals. In Griffin, D. R. (ed.). Animal Mind — Human Mind. Springer-Verlag: Berlin.

Drew, G. C. (1938). The function of punishment in learning. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 52, 257—67.

Dunham, P. J. (1968). Contrasted conditions of reinforcement: a selective critique. Psychological Bulletin, 69, 295- 315.

Dunlap, K., Gentry, E. and Zeigler,J. W. (1931). The behaviour of white rats under food and electric shock stimulation. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12, 37 1-8.

Dworkin, B. R. and Miller, N. E. (1986). Failure to replicate visceral learning in the acute curarized rat preparation. Behavioural Neuroscience, 100, 299—314.

Eikelboom, R. and Stewart, J. (1982). The conditioning of drug- induced physiological response. Psychological Review, 89, 507—28.

Elliot, M. H. (1928). The effect of change of reward on the maze performance of rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 19—30.

Ellison, G. D. (1964). Differential salivary conditioning to traces. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 57, 373—80.

Emmerton, J. (1983). Vision. In Abs, M. (ed.). Physiology and Behaviour of the Pigeon. Academic Press: London, 245—66.

Epstein, R. Kirschnit, C. E., Lanza, R. P. and Rubin, L. C. (1984). Insight in the pigeon: antecedents and determinants of an intelligent performance. Nature, 308, 6 1—2.

Erlich, A. (1970). Response to novel objects in 3 lower primates: greater galago, slow-loris, and owl monkey. Behaviour, 37, 55—63.

Ernst, A. J., Engerg, L. and Thomas, D. R. (1971). On the forms of stimulus generalization curves for visual intensity. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 16, 177—80.

Estes, W. K. (1944). An experimental study of punishment. Psychological Monographs, 57, no. 3.

Evans, S. (1936). The role of kinaesthesis in the establishment and control of the maze habit. Journal of Genetic Psychologys, 48, 177—98.

Ewert, J.-P. (1976). The visual system of the toad: behavioural and physiological studies of a pattern recognition system. In Fite, K. V. (ed.). The Amphibian Visual System. Academic Press: New York, 141—202.

Ewert, J -P. (1982). Neuronal basis of configurational prey selection by the common toad. In Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A. and Mansfield, R. J. W. (eds.). Analysis of Visual Behaviour. MIT Press: Cambridge, Mass., 1—45.

Eysenck, H. J. (1975). A note on backward conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 13, 201.

Eysenck, H. J. (1976). The learning theory model of neurosis — a new approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 14, 251—67.

Eysenck, M. W. (1977). Human Memory: Theory, Research and Individual Differences. Pergamon: Oxford.

Fantino, E. (1967). Preference for mixed versus fixed-ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 10, 35—43.

Fantino, E. and Logan, C. (1979). The Experimental Analysis of Behaviour: A Biological Perspective. W. H. Freeman; San Francisco.

Feldman, J. A. (1985). Four frames suffice: a provisional model of vision and space. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 8, 265—89.

Ferrier, D. (1878). The Localization of Cerebral Disease. Smith Elder: London. Ferster, C. B. and Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of Reinforcement. Appleton Century- Crofts: New York.

Finch, G. (1938). Salivary conditioning in atropinized dogs. American Journal of Physiology, 124, 136—41.

Fisher, G.J. (1962). The formation of learning sets in young gorillas. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55, 924—5.

Fodor, J. A. (1980). Fixation of belief and concept acquisition. In PiatelliPalmarini, M. (ed.). Language and Learning: The Debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, Mass.

Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind.. MIT Press: London. Fowler, H. and Wischner, G. J. (1969). The varied functions of punishment in discrimination learning. In Campbell, B. A. and Church, R. M. (eds.). Punishment and Aversive Behaviour. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, 375- 420.

Fraenkel, G. S. and Gunn, D. L. (1940). The Orientation of Animals: Kineses, Taxes and Compass Reactions. Clarendon Press, Oxford.

Fragaszy, D. M. and Mason, W. A. (1983). Comparisons of feeding behaviour in captive squirrel and Titi monkeys (Saimiri sciureus and Callicebus moloch). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 97, 310—26.

Freud, 5. (1936). The Problem of Anxiety. Norton: New York.

Freud, S. (1959). Inhibitions, symptoms and anxiety. In Strachey,J. (ed.). The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud. Vol. XX. Hogarth Press: London, 87—1 74.

Frisby, J. P. (1979). Seeing. Oxford University Press.

Furness, W. H. (1916). Observations on the mentality of chimpanzees and orangutaρs. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 65, 28 1—90.

Gaffan, D. (1974). Recognition impaired and association inact in the memory of monkeys after transection of the fornix. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 1 100—9.

Gaffan, D. (1977a). Discrimination of word-like compound visual stimuli by monkeys. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 589—96.

Gaffan, D. (1977b). Monkeys’ recognition memory for complex pictures and the effect of fornix transection. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 505—14.

Gaffan, D. (1977c). Recognition memory after short retention intervals in fornix-transected monkeys. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 577—8.

Gaffan, D. (1983). A comment on primacy effects in monkeys’ memory lists. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 11, 144—5.

Gaffan, D. (1985). Hippocampus: memory, habit and voluntary movement. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 308, 87—99.

Gaffan, D., Saunders, R. C., Gaffan, E. A., Harrison, S., Shields, C. and Owen, M. J. (l984a). Effects of fornix transaction upon associative memory in monkeys: role of hippocampus in learned action. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36B, 173—221.

Gaffan, D., Gaffan, E. A. and Harrison, S. (l984b). Effects of fornix transection on spontaneous and trained non-matching by monkeys. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36B, 285—303.

Gaffan, D. Shields, C., and Harrison, S. (1984c). Delayed matching by fornix-transected monkeys: the sample, the push and the bait. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36B, 305—17.

Garcia, J. (1981). Tilting at the paper mills of academe. American Psychologist, 36, 149—58.

Garcia, J. and Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4, 123—4.

Garcia,J., Ervin, F., Yorke, C. and Koelling, R. (1967). Conditioning with delayed vitamin injections. Science, 155, 7 16-18.

Garcia, J., Hankins, W. and Rusiniak. K. (1974). Behavioural regulation of the milieu interne in man and rat. Science, 185, 824-3 1.

Garcia, J., Rusinak, K. W., and Brett, L. P. (1977b). Conditioning food-aversions in wild animals: caveant canonici. In Davis, H. and Hurwitz, H. M. B. (eds), Operant-Pavlovian Interactions. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, 273—316.

Garcia,J., Hankins, W. G. and Coil,J. D. (1977a). Koalas, men and other conditioned gastronomes. In Milgram, N. W., Krames, L. and Alloway, T. M. (eds). Food Aversion Learning. Plenum Press: London, 195-2 18.

Gardner, R. A. and Gardner, B. T. (1969). Teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. Science, 165, 664—72.

Gardner, B. T. and Gardner, R. A. (1971). Two-way communication with an infant chimpanzee. In Schrier, A. M. and Stollnitz, F. (eds). Behaviour of Nonhuman Primates. Vol. 4. Academic Press: New York, 117-83.

Gardner, B. T. and Gardner, R. A. (1985). Signs of intelligence in cross-fostered chimpanzees. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 308, 159—176.

Gibbon, J. (1977). Scalar expectancy theory and Weber’s law in animal timing. Psychological Review, 84, 279—325.

Gibson, E. J. and Walk, R. D. (1956). The effect of prolonged exposure to visually presented patterns of learning to discriminate them. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 49, 239—42.

Gillan, D. J. (1981). Reasoning in the chimpanzee: II. Transitive inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 7, 150—64.

Gillan, D. J., Premack, D. and Woodruff, G. (1981). Reasoning in the chimpanzee: I. Analogical reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 7, 1—17.

Glazer, H. I. and Weiss, J. M. (1976). Long-term interference effect: an alternative to ‘Learned Helplessness’. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 2, 202—13.

Glickman, S. E. and Schiff, B. B. (1967). A biological theory of reinforcement. Psychological Review, 74, 81—109.

Goodlass, A. and Geschwind, N. (1976). Language disorders (aphasia). In Carterette, E. C. and Friedman, M. P. (eds). Handbook of Perception, Vol. VII. Academic Press: London, 389—428.

Goodkin, F. (1976). Rats learn the relationship between responding and environmental events: an expansion of the learned helplessness hypothesis. Learning and Motivation, 7, 382—93.

Gormezano. I. (1965). Yoked comparisons of classical and instrumental conditioning of the eyelid response; and an addendum on ‘voluntary responders’. In Prokasy, W. F. (ed). Classical Conditioning: A Symposium Appleton-Century- Crofts: New York, 48—70.

Goss-Custard, J. D. (1977). The energetics of prey selection by redshank, Tringa totanus (L.), in relation to prey density. Journal of Animal Ecology, 46, 1—19.

Gossette, R. L. and Hood, P. (1968). Successive discrimination reversal measures as a function of variation of motivational and incentive levels. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 47—52.

Gossette, R. L., Gossette, M. F. and Riddell, W. (1966). Comparisons of successive discrimination reversal performances among loosely and remotely related avian species. Animal Behaviour, 14, 560—4.

Gossette, R. L., Kraus, G. and Speiss, J. (1968). Comparison of successive discrimination reversal (SDR) performances of seven mammalian species on a spatial task. Psychonomic Science, 12, 193—4.

Gottlieb, G. (1984). Evolutionary trends and evolutionary origins: relevance to theory in comparative psychology. Psychological Review, 91, 448—56.

Grant, D. G., Brewster, R. G. and Stierhoff, K. A. (1983). ‘Surprisingness’ and short-term retention in the pigeon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 9, 63—79.

Grant, D. S. (1976). Effect of sample presentation time on long-delay matching in the pigeon. Learning and Motivation,:I7, 580—90.

Grant, D. S. (1981). Short-term memory in the pigeon. In Spear, N. E. and Miller, R. R. (eds). Information Processing in Animals, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 226—56.

Grant, D. S. and Roberts, W. A. (1976). Sources of retroactive inhibition in pigeon short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 2, 1—16.

Gray, J. A. (1975). Elements of a Process Theory of Learning. Academic Press: London.

Gray, J. A. (1979). Pavlov. Fontaria: London.

Gray, J. A. (1982). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-Hippocampal System. Oxford University Press.

Gray, J. A. (1984). The hippocampus as an interface between cognition and emotion. In Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London.

Green, K. F. and Garcia, J. (1971). Recuperation from illness: flavour enhancement for rats. Science, 173, 249—51.

Green, S. (1987). An Introduction to Physiological Psychology. Routledge & Kegan Paul: London.

Green, S. and Marler, P. (1979). The analysis of animal communication. In Marler, P. and Vandenbergh, J~ G. (eds). Handbook of Behavioural Neurobiology, Vol. 3. Plenum Press: London, 73—158.

Greene, S. (1983). Feature memorization in pigeon concept formation. In Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J. and Wagner, A. R. (eds). Quantititive Analyses of Behaviour: Discrimination Processes. Balinger: Cambridge, Mass., 209—29.

Gregg, V. (1986). An Introduction to Human Memory. Routledge & Kegan, Paul: London.

Griffin, D. R. (1984). Animal Thinking. Harvard University Press: London. Grossberg, S. (1982). Processing of expected and unexpected events during conditioning and attention: a psychophysiological theory. Psychological Review, 89, 529—72.

Groves, P. M. and Thompson, R. F. (1970). Habituation: a dual- process theory. Psychological Reivew, 77, 419—50.

Gruber, H. E., Girgus, J. S. and Banuazizi, A. (1971). The development of object permanence in the cat. Developmental Psychology, 4, 9—15.

Grzimek, B. (1974). Animal Life Encyclopedia, Vol.5. Van Nostrand: London.       

Gustavson, C. R., Garcia, J., Hankins, W. G. and Rusiniak, K. W. (1974). Coyote predation control by aversive conditioning. Science, 184, 58 1—3.

Guttman, N. and Kalish, H. I. (1956). Discriminability and stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 79—88.

Hailman, J. P. (1962). Pecking of laughing gull chicks at models of the parental head. Auk, 79, 89—98.

Haley, T. J. and Snyder, R. S. (eds). (1964). The Response of the Nervous System to Ionizing Radiation. Little Brown: Boston.

Halgren, C. R. (1974). Latent inhibition in rats: associative or non-associative. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 74—8.

Hall, G. (1980). Exposure learning in animals. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 535—50.

Hall, G. (1983). Behaviour. Academic Press: London.

Hall, G. and Pearce, J. M. (1979). Latent inhibition of a CS during CS-US pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 5, 31—42.

Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical theory of social behaviour. I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1—32.

Hammond, L. J. (1980). The effect of contingency upon the appetitive conditioning of free-operant behaviour. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 34, 297—304.

Hankins, W. G., Garcia, J. and Rusiniak, K. (1973). Dissociation of odour and taste in baitshyness. Behavioural Biology, 8, 407—19.

Hankins, W. G., Rusiniak, K. W. and Garcia, J. (1976). Dissociation of odor and taste in shock avoidance learning. Behavioural Biology, 18, 345—58.

Hanson, H. M. (1959). Effects of discrimination training on stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 321—34.

Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56, 51—65.

Harlow, H. F. (1950). Analysis of discrimination learning by monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 26—39.

Harlow, H. F. (1958). The nature of love. American Psychologist, 13, 673—85. Harlow, H. F. (1959). Learning set and error factor theory. In Koch, S. (ed.). Psychology: A Study of a Science. Vol., 2. McGraw-Hill: New York, 492—537.

Harlow, H. F. (1962). The heterosexual affectional system in monkeys. American Psychologist, 17, 1—9.

Harlow, H. F. and Harlow, M. K. (1965). The affectional systems. In Schrier, A., Harlow, H. F. and Stollnitz, F. (eds). Behaviour of Nonhuman Primates, Vol. 2. Academic Press: London, 287—334.

Harlow, H. F. and Suomi, S. J. (1970). The nature of love simplified. American Psychologist, 25, 161—8.

Harlow, H. F., Harlow, M. K. and Meyer, D. R. (1950). Learning motivated by a manipulation drive. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 228—34.

Harris, J. D. (1943). Studies on non-associative factors inherent in conditioning. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 18(1), 1—74.

Harris, M. and Coltheart, M. (1986). Language Processing in Children and Adults. Routledge & Kegan Paul: London.

Harrison, J. M. (1978). Functional properties of the auditory system of the brain stem. In Masterton, R. B. (ed.). Handbook of Behavioural Neurobiology, Vol. L Plenum Press: London, 409-458.

Hawkins, R. D. and Kandel, E. R. (1984). Is there a cell- biological alphabet for simple forms of learning?. Psychological Review, 91, 375—91.

Hawkins, R. D., Abrams, T. W., Carew, T. J. and Kandel, E. R. (1983). A cellular mechanism for classical conditioning in Aplysia: Activity-dependent amplification of pre- synaptic facilitation. Science, 219, 400—5.

Hayes, K. J. and Hayes, C. (1951). The intellectual development of a home raised chimpanzee. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 95, 105—9.

Hayes, K. J. and Hayes, C. (1953). Picture perception in a home raised chimpanzee. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 46, 470-4.

Herrnstein, R. J. and de Villiers, P.A. (1980) Fish as a natural category for people and pigeons. In Bower, G. H. (ed.). The Psychology of Learning and Motivation. Vol. 14. Academic Press: New York, 60—97.

Herrnstein, R. J. and Hineline, P. N. (1966). Negative reinforcement as shock frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 9, 42 1—30.

Herrnstein, R. J. and Loveland, D. H. (1964). Complex visual concept in the pigeon. Science, 146, 549—5 1.

Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H. and Cable, C. (1976). Natural concepts in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Animal Behaviour Processes, 2, 285—302.

Hess, E. H. (1959). Imprinting. Science, 130, 133—41.

Heth, C. D. (1976). Simultaneous and backward fear conditioning as a function of number of CS-US pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 2, 117—29.

Heth, C. D. and Rescorla, R. A. (1973). Simultaneous and backward fear conditioning in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 434—43.

Hinde, R. A. (1960). Energy models of motivation. Symposia of the Society for Experimental Biology, 14, 199—213.

Hinde, R. A. (1970). Animal Behaviour: A Synthesis of Ethology and Comparative Psychology. McGraw-Hill: London.

Hinde, R. A. and Stevenson-Hinde, J. (eds). (1973). Constraints on Learning. Academic Press: London.

Hodos, W. (1961). Progressive ratio as a measure of reward strength. Science, 134, 943—4.

Hodos, W. and Bonbright, J. C. (1972). The detection of visual intensity differences by pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 18, 471—9.

Hodos, W. and Campbell, C. B. G. (1969). Scale Naturae: why there is no theory of comparative psychology. Psychological Review, 76, 337—50.

Hodos, W. and Kalman, G. (1963). Effects of increment size and reinforcer volume on progressive ratio performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 6, 387—92.

Holding, D. H. and Jones, D. D. (1976). Delayed one-trial extinction of the McCullough effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21, 683—7.

Holland, P. C. (1977). Conditioned stimulus as a determinant of the form of the Pavlovian conditioned response. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 3, 77—104.

Holland, P. C., and Rescorla, R. A. (1975). Second-order conditioning with food unconditioned stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 459—67.

Holman, E. W. (1975). Some conditions for the dissociation of consummatory and instrumental behaviour in rats. Learning and Motivation, 6, 358—66.

Holman, G. L. (1968). Intra gastric reinforcement effect. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 69, 432—41.

Honig, W. K. (1978). Studies of working memory in the pigeon. In Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H. and Honig, W. K. (eds). Cognitive Processes in Animal Behaviour. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 211-48.

Honig, W. K. (1981). Working memory and the temporal map. In Spear, N. E. and Miller, R. R. (eds). lnformation Processing in Animals. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 167—97.

Honig, W. K. (~984). Contributions of animal memory to the interpretation of animal learning. In Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, 29—44.

Honzik, C. H. (1933). Maze learning in rats in the absence of specific intra— and extra-maze stimuli. University of California Publications in Psychology, 6, 99—144.

Honzik, C. H. (1936). The sensory basis of maze learning in rats. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 13, 1—113.

Horn, G. (1967). Neuronal mechanisms of habituation. Nature, 215, 707—11.

Horowitz. L. M., Lippman, L. G., Norman, S. A. and McConkie, G.W. (1964). Compound stimuli in paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 132—141.

Horridge, G. A. (1962). Learning of leg position by the ventral nerve cord in headless insects. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 157, 33—52.

Howard, J. M. and Campion, R. C. (1978). A metric for pattern discrimination performance. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics, 8, 32—7.

Hudson, B. B. (1939). One-trial learning in rats. Psychological Bulletin, 36, 642.

Hudson, B. B. (1950). One-trial learning in the domestic rat. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 41, 99—145.

Hull, C. L. (1934). The concept of habit-family hierarchy and maze learning. Psychological Review, 41, 33—54.

Hull, C. L. (1937). Mind, mechanism and adaptive behaviour. Psychological Review, 44, 1—32.

Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of Behaviour. Appleton- Century-Crofts: New York.

Hull, C. L. (1952). A Behaviour System. Yale University Press: New Haven. Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H. and Honig, W. K. (eds). (1978). Cognitive Process in Animal Behaviour. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey.

Hume, D. (1777/1970). Enquiries Concerning the Human Understanding and Concerning the Principles of Morals. Oxford University Press: London.

Humphrey, G. (1930). Le Chatelier’s rule and the problem of habituation and dehabituation in Helix albolaris. Psychologiche Forschung, 13, 113-27.

Humphrey, G. (1933). The Nature of Learning in Relation to Living Systems. Kegan Paul, Trench & Trubner: London.

Hunter, M. W. and Kamil, A. L. (1971). Object-discrimination learning set and hypothesis behaviour in the Northern Bluejay (Cyanocitta cristata). Psychonomic Science, 22, 27 1—3.

Hunter, W. S. (1930). A consideration of Lashley’s theory of the equipotentiality of cerebral action. Journal of General Psychology, 3, 455—68.

Hurvich, L. M. and Jameson, D. (1974). Opponent process as a model of neural organization. American Psycholgist, 29, 88—102.

Hurwitz, H. M. B. (1955). Response elimination without performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 7, 1—7.

Ince, L. P. Brucker, B. S. and Alba, A. (1978). Reflex conditioning of a spinal man. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 92, 796—802.

Ingebritsen, 0. C. (1932). Maze learning after lesion in the cervical cord. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 14, 279—94.

Ingle, D. J. (1982). Organization of visuomotor behaviours in vertebrates. In Ingle, D. J., Goodale, M. A. and Mansfield, R. J. W. (eds). Analysis of Visual Behaviour. MIT Press; London, 67—109.

Jackson, J. H. (1888/1931). Selected Writings. Vol. 1. J. Taylor, ed. Hodder & Stoughton: London.

Jackson, R. L., Alexander, J. H. and Maier, S. F. (1980). Learned helplessness, inactivity, and associative deficits: effects of inescapable shock on response choice escape learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 6, 1—20.

James, W. (1890/1983). The Principles of Psychology. Harvard University Press: London.

Jassik-Gerschenfeld, D., Lange, R. V. and Robert, N. (1977). Response of movement detecting cells in the optic tectum of pigeons to change of wavelength. Vision Research, 17, 1139—46.

Jeffrey, W. E. (1968). The orienting reflex in cognitive development. Psychological Review, 75, 323—34.

Jeffrey, W. E. and Cohen, L. B. (1971). Habituation in the human infant. In Reese, H. (ed). Advances in Child Development and Behaviour. Academic Press: New York, 63—97.

Jenkins, H. M. (1977). Sensitivity of different response systems to stimulus-reinforcer and response-reinforcer relations. In Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. and Gibbon, J. (eds). Autoshaping and Conditioning Theory. Academic Press: London, 47—66.

Jenkins, H. M. and Harrison, R. H. (1960). Effect of discrimination training on auditory generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 246—53.

Jenkins, H. M. and Moore, B. R. (1973). The form of the autoshaped

Kant, 1. (178 1/1979). Critique of Pure Reason. Everyman’s Library, Dent: London.

Karli, P. (1956). The Norway rat’s killing response to the white mouse; an experimental anaylsis. Behaviour, 10, 8 1—103.

Karten, H. J. (1979). Visual leminiscal pathways in birds. In Granda, A. M. and Maxwell, J. H. (eds). Neural Mechanisms of Behaviour in the Pigeon. Plenum: New York, 409—SO.

Kaye, H. and Pearce, J. M. (1984). The strength of the orienting response during Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 10, 90—109.

Keith-Lucas, J. and Guttman, N. (1975). Robust single-trial delayed backward conditioning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 468—76.

Kelleher, R. T. and Gollub, L. R. (1962). A review of positive conditional reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 5, 543—97.

Kellogg, W. N. (1947). Is ‘spinal conditioning’ conditioning? Reply to ‘A comment’. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 264—5.

Kellogg, W. N. (1968). Communication and language in the home- raised chimpanzee. Science, 162, 423—7.

Kellogg, W. N. and Kellogg, L. A. (1933/1967). The Ape and the Child: A Study of Envrionmental Influence on Early Behaviour. Haffner: New York.

Kesner, R. P. and Cook, D. G. (1983). Role of habituation and classical conditioning in the development of morphine tolerance. Behavioural Neuroscience, 97, 4—12.

Kesner, R. P. and Novak, J. M. (1982). Serial position curve in rats: role of the dorsal hippocampus. Science, 218, 173—5.

Kimble, G. A. (1961). Hilgard and Marquis’ Conditioning and Learning. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York.

Kintsch, W. (1970). Learning, Memory and Conceptual Processes. Wiley: London.

Klinghammer, E. (1967). Factors influencing choice of mate in altricial birds. In Stevenson, H. W., Hess, E. H. and Rheingold, H. L. (eds), Early Behaviour. Wiley: New York.

Kohler, W. (1925). The Mentality of Apes. Kegan Paul, Trench & Trubner: London.

Kohler, W. (1929). Gestalt Psychology. Loveright: New York.

Konorski, J. (1948). Conditioned Reflexes and Neuron Organization. Cambridge University Press.

Konorski, J. (1967). lnteractive Activity of the Brain. University of Chicago Press.

Konorski, J. and Miller, S. (1937). On two types of conditioned reflex. Journal of General Psychology, 16, 264—72.

Krane, R. V. and Wagner, A. R. (1975). Taste aversion learning with a delayed shock US: implications for the ‘Generality of the laws of learning’. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 882—9.

Krebs, J. R. (1979). Foraging strategies and their social significance. In Marler, P. and Vandenbergh, J. G. (eds). Handbook of Behavioural Neurobiology: Vol. 3. Social Behaviour and Communication. Plenum Press: New York, 225—70.

Krebs, J. R. and Davies, N. B. (eds). (1983). Behavioural Ecology. An Evolutionary Approach. 2nd ed. Blackwell Scientific Publications: Oxford.

Krebs, J. R., MacRoberts, M. H. and Cullen J. M. (1972). Flocking and feeding in the great tit: an experimental study. Ibis. 114, 505—30.

Krebs,J. R., Kacelnik, A. and Taylor, P. (1978). Tests of optimal sampling by foraging great tits. Nature, 275, 27—31.

Krechevsky, I. (1932). ‘Hypotheses’ in rats. Psychological Review, 39, 5 16-32. Krechevsky, I. (1938). A study of the continuity of the problem-solving process. Psychological Review, 45, 107—33.

Kriekhaus, E. E. and Wolf, G. (1968). Acquisition of sodium by rats: interaction of innate mechanisms and latent learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 65, 197—201.

Krueger, R. C. and Hull, C. L. (1931). An electro-chemical parallel to the conditioned reflex. Journal of General Psychology, 5, 262—9.

Kruse, J. M., Overmeier, J. B., Konz, W. A. and Rokke, E. (1983). Pavlovian conditioned stimulus effects upon instrumental choice behaviour are reinforcer specific. Learning and Motivation, 14,, 165—81.

Kuhl, P. K. and Miller, J. D. (1975). Speech perception by the Chinchilla: voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants. Science, 190, 69—72.

Kuo, Z. Y. (1924). A psychology without heredity. Psychological Review, 31, 427—51.

Kuo, Z. Y. (1932). Ontogeny of embryonic behaviour. IV: The influence of embryonic movements on behaviour after hatching. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 14, 109—22.

Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence. University of Chicago Press.

Lashley, K. 5. (1938). The mechanism of vision: XV. Preliminary studies of the rat’s capacity for detail vision. Journal of General Psychology, 18, 123—93.

Lashley, K. S. (1942). An examination of the ‘continuity theory’ as applied to discrimination learning. Journal of General Psychology, 26, 241—65.

Lashley, K. S. and Ball, J. (1929). Spinal conduction and kinaesthetic sensitivity in the maze habit. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 9, 71—106.

Lawrence, D. H. (1949). Acquired distinctiveness of cues: I. Transfer between discriminations on the basis of familiarity with the stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 770—84.

Lawrence, D. H. (1950). Acquired distinctiveness of cues: II. Selective association in a constant stimulus situation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40, 175—88.

Lawrence, D. H. (1952). The transfer of a discrimination along a continuum. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 45, 511-6.

Lea, S. E. G. (1978). The psychology and economics of demand. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 441—66.

Lea, S. E. G. (1979). Foraging and reinforcement schedules in the pigeon: optimal and non-optimal aspects of choice. Animal Behaviour, 27, 875—86. Lea, S. E. G. (1981). Correlation and contiguity in foraging behaviour. In Harzem, P. and Zeiler, M. D. (eds). Predictability, Correlation and Contiguity. Wiley: Chichester, 355—406.

Lea, S. E. G. (1984a). Instinct, Environment and Behaviour. Methuen: London. Lea, S. E. G. (1984b). In what sense do pigeons learn concepts?. In Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 263—76.

Lea, S. E. 0. and Ryan, C. (1983). Feature analysis of pigeons’ acquisition of concept discrimination. In Commons, M. L., Herrnstein, R. J. and Wagner, A. R. (eds). Quantititive Analyses of Behaviour: Discriimination Processes. Ballinger: Cambridge, Mass., 239—53.

Leger, D. W., Owings, D. H. and Coss, R. G. (1983). Behavioural ecology of time allocation in California ground squirrels (Spermophilus beecheyi): microhabitat effects. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 97, 283-9 1.

Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. Wiley: New York.

Lett. B. T. and Harley, C. W. (1974). Stimulation of lateral hypothalamus during sickness attenuates learning flavour aversions. Physiology and Behaviour, 12, 79—83.

Levey, A. B. and Martin, I. (1975). Classical conditioning of human evaluative response. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 13, 22 1-6.

Levey, A. B. and Martin, I. (1983). Cognitions, evaluation and conditioning: rules of sequence and rules of consequence. Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy, 4, 18 1—95.

Lieberman, P. (1975). On the Origins of Language. Macmillan: New York.

Lieberman, P. (1977). The phylogeny of language. In Sebeok, T. A. (ed.). How Animals Communicate. Indiana University Press: Bloomington, 3—25.

Lockard, J. A. (1963). Choice of a warning signal or no warning signal in an unavoidable shock situation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 526—30.

Locke, J. (1689/1982). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. Clarendon Press: Oxford.

Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. and Gibbon, J. (1976). Autoshaping, random control, and omission training in the rat. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 26, 45 1-62.

Logan, F. A. (1969). The negative incentive value of punishment. In Campbell, B. A. and Church, R. M. (eds). Punishment and Aversive Behaviour. Appleton-Century- Crofts: New York, 43—56.

Logue, A. W. (1979). Taste aversion and the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 276—96.

Lore, R. and Flannelly, K. (1978). Habitat selection and burrow construction by wild Rattus norvegicus in a landfill. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 92, 888—96.

Lorenz, K. (1967). On Agression. Methuen: London.

Lorge, I. and Sells, S. B. (1936). Representative factors in the rat under ‘changed-incentive’ techinique. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 49, 479—80.

Lovibond, P. F., Preston, G. C. and Mackintosh, N. J. (1984). Context specificity of conditioning, extinction, and latent inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 10, 360—75.

Lowe, C. F. (1983). Radical behaviourism and human psychology. In Davey, G. C. L. (ed.). Animal Models of Human Behaviour. Wiley: Chichester, 7 1—93.

Lumsden, C. J. and Wilson, E. 0. (1981). Genes, Mind and Culture: The Coevolutionary Process. Harvard University Press: Cambridge Mass.

McCoy, D. F. and Lange, K. 0. (1969). Stimulus generalization of gravity. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 12, 111—18.

McCullough, C. (1965). Colour adaptation of edge detectors in the human visual system. Science, 149, 115-16.

Mackintosh, N. J. (1969). Further analysis of the overtraining reversal effect. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. Monographs, 67, no. 2, part 2.

Mackintosh, N. J. (1973). Stimulus selection: learning to ignore stimuli that predict no change in reinforcement. In Hinde, R. A. and Stevenson Hinde, J. (eds). Constraints on Learning. Academic Press: London, 75—96.

Mackintosh, N.J. (1974). The Psychology of Animal Learning. Academic Press: London.

Mackintosh, N.J. (1975). A theory of attention: variations in associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276—98.

Mackintosh, N. J. (1983). Conditioning and Associative Learning. Clarendon Press:Oxford.

Mackintosh, N. J., Wilson, B. and Boakes, R. A. (1985). Differences in mechanisms of intelligence among vertebrates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 308, 53—65.

Macphail, E. M. (1982). Brain and Intellegince in Vertebrates. Clarendon Press: Oxford.

Macphail, E. M. (1985). Vertebrate intelligence: the null hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 308, 37—51.

MacFarlane, D. A. (1930). The role of kinaesthesis in maze learning. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 277—305.

Maier, S. F. (1970). Failure to escape traumatic electric shock: incompatable skeletal-motor responses or learned helplessness. Learning and Motivation, 1, 157—69.

Maier, S. F. and Seligman, M. E. P. (1976). Learned helplessness: theory and evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology, General, 105, 3—46.

Maier, S. F. and Testa, T. J. (1975). Failure to learn to escape by rats previously exposed to shock is partly produced by associative interference. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 554—64.

Maier, S. F., Seligman, M. E. P. and Solomon, R. L. (1969). Pavlovian fear conditioning and learned helplessness. In Campbell, B. A. and Church, R. M. (eds). Punishment and Aversive Behaviour. Appleton-CenturyCrofts: New York, 299—342.

Maier, S. F. ShermanmJ. E., Lewis,J. W., Terman, G. W. and Liebeskind, J. C. (1983). The opoidnonopoid nature of stress- induced analgesia and learned helplessness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 9, 80—90.

Maki, W. S. (1981). Directed forgetting in animals. In Spear, N. E. and Miller, R. R. (eds). Information Processing in Animals. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 199—225.

Mallott, R. and Sidall, J. (1972). Acquisition of the people concept in pigeons. Psychological Reports, 31, 3—13.

Marler, P. (1970). A comparative approach to vocal learning: song development in white-crowned sparrows. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology Monograph, 71, Pt. 2, 1—25.

Marler, P. and Waser, M. S. (1977). Role of auditory feedback in canary song development. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91, 8—16.

Marr, D. (1982). Vision. W. H. Freeman: San Francisco.

Marsh, G. (1969). An evaluation of three explanations for the transfer of discrimination effect. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 68,268—75.

Marwine, A. and Collier, G. (1979). The rat at the water hole. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 93, 391-402.

Maslach, C. (1979). Negative emotional biasing of unexplained arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 953—69.

Mason, W. A. (1967). Motivational aspects of social responsiveness in young chimpanzees. In Stevenson, H. W., Hess, E. H. and Rheingold, H. L. (eds). Early Behaviour. Wiley: London, 103—26.

Mason, W. A. (1979). Ontogeny of social behaviour. In Marler, P. and Vandenburgh, J. G. (eds). Handbook of Behavioural Neurobiology. Vol. 3. Social Behaviour and Communication. Plenum Press: London, 1—28.

Masterton, R. B. and Glendenning, K. K. (1979). Phylogeny of the vertebrate sensory systems. In Masterton, R. B. (ed.). Handbook of Behavioural Neurology. Vol. 1. Plenum: New York, 1—38.

Matsuzawa, T. (1985). Use of numbers by a chimpanzee. Nature, 315, 57—9. May, J. G. and Matteson, H. H. (1976). Spatial frequency contingent colour after effects. Science, 192, 145—6.

Maynard-Smith, J. (1975). The Theory of Evolution. Penguin: Harmondsworth.

Maynard-Smith, J. (1984). Game theory and the evolution of behaviour. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 7, 95—126.

Mechner, F. (1958). Probability relations within response sequences under ratio reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 1, 109-21.

Meck, W. H. and Church, R. M. (1984). Simultaneous temporal processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 10, 1—29.

Meck, W. H., Church, R. M. and Olton, D. S. (1984). Hippocampus, time, and memory. Behavioural Neuroscience, 98, 3—22.

Medin, D. L., Roberts, W. A. and Davis, R. T. (eds). (1976). Processes of Animal Memory Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey.

Melgren, R. L. and Ost, J. W. P. (1969). Transfer of Pavlovian differential conditioning to an operant discrimination. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 67, 390-4.

Melvin, K. B. and Smith, F. H. (1967). Self-punitive avoidance behaviour in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 533—5.

Mendelson, J. (1966). Role of hunger in T-maze learning for food by rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 62, 341—9.

Menzel, E. W. (1973). Chimpanzee spatial memory organization. Science, 182, 943—5.

Menzel, E. W. (1978). Cognitive mapping in chimpanzees. In Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H. and Honig, W. K. (eds), Cognitive Processes in Animal Behaviour. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NewJersey, 375—422.

Menzel, E. W. and Juno, C. (1985). Social foraging in marmoset monkeys and the question of intelligence. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 308, 145—58.

Milgram, N. W., Krames, L. and Alloway, T. M. (1977). Food Aversion Learning. Plenum Press: New York.

Miller, I. W. and Norman, W. H. (1979). Learned helplessness in humans: a review and attribution theory model. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 93—118.

Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In Hunt, J. McV. (ed.). Personality and the Behaviour Disorders. Vol. L Ronald Press: New York, 431-65.

Miller, N. E. (1948). Studies of fear as an acquirable drive. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 89-101.

Miller, N. E. (1969). Learning of visceral and glandular responses. Science, 163, 434—44.

Miller, N. E. (1978). Biofeedback and visceral learning. Annual Review of Psychology, 29, 373—404.

Miller, N. E. and Dicara, L. (1967). Instrumental learning of heart-rate changes in curarized rats: shaping, and specificity to discrimininative stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 12—19.

Miller, N. E. and Weiss, J. M. (1969). Effects of the somatic or visceral responses to punishment. In Campbell, B. A. and Church, R. M. (eds). Punishment and Aversive Behaviour. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, 343—72.

Miller, R. R. and Balaz, M. A. (1981). Differences in adaptiveness between classically conditioned and instrumentally acquired responses. In Spear, N. E. and Miller, R. R. (eds). Information Processing in Animals. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 49—80.

Miller, R. R., Greco, C., Vigorito, M. and Marlin, N. A. (1983). Signalled tailshock is perceived as similar to a stronger unsignalled tailshock: implications for a functional analysis of classical conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 9, 105—31.

Miller, V. and Domjan, M. (1981). Specificity of cue to consequence in aversion learning in the rat: control for US- induced differential orientations. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 9, 339—45.

Milner, B., Corkin, S. and Teuber, H.-L. (1968). Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14 year follow- up study of H. M. Neuropsychologia, 6, 215—34.

Miranda, S. B. and Fanz, R. L. (1974). Recognition memory in Down’s syndrome and normal children. Child Development, 45, 65 1-60.

Mishkin, M. (1978). Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not separate removal of amygdala and hippocampus. Nature, 273, 297—8.

Mishkin, M. (1982). A memory system in the monkey. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B. 298. 85—95.

Moore, B. R. (1973). The role of directed Pavlovian reactions in simple instrumental learning in the pigeon. In Hinde, R. A. and StevensonHinde, J. (eds). Constraints on Learning. Academic Press: London, 159—86.

Morgan, C. L. (1894). An Introduction to Comparative Psychology. Walter Stott: London.

Morgan, M. J. (1974). Do rats like to work for their food?. Learning and Motivation, 5, 352-6.

Morgan, M. J. (1979). Motivational processes. In Dickinson, A. and Boakes, R. A. (eds). Mechanisms of Learning and Motivation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 171—201.

Morgan, M. J. and Nicholas, D. J. (1979). Discrimination between reinforced action patterns in the rat. Learning and Motivation, 10, 1—27.

Morgan, M. J., Fitch, M. D., Holman, J. G. and Lea, S. E. G. (1976). Pigeons learn the concept of an ‘A’. Perception, 5, 57-66.

Morris, R.. G. M. (1975). Preconditioning of reinforcing properties to an exteroceptive feedback stimulus. Learning and Motivation, 6, 289—98.

Morris, R. G. M. (1981). Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learning and Motivation, 12, 239—60.

Morris, R. G. M., Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N. P. and O’Keefe, J. (1982). Place navigation impaired in rats with hippocampal lesions. Nature, 297, 681—3.

Morrow, J. 0. and Smithson, B. L. (1969). Learning sets in an invertebrate. Science, 164, 850—1.

Morse, W. A. and Kelleher, R. T. (1977). Determinants of reinforcement and punishment. In Honig, W. K. and Staddon,J. E. R. (eds), Handbook of Operant Behaviour. Prentice- Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ, I 74—200.

Morse, P. A. and Snowdon, C. J. (1975). An investigation of categorical speech discrimination by rhesus monkeys. Perception and Psychopysics, 17, 9—16.

Mowrer, 0. H. (1939). A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Psychological Review, 46, 553—65.

Mowrer, 0. H. (1940). Anxiety reduction and learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27, 497—5 16.

Mowrer, 0. H. (1947). On the dual nature of learning — a reinterpretation of ‘conditioning’ and ‘problem solving’. Harvard Educational Review, 17, 102-48.

Mowrer, 0. H. (1960). Learning Theory and Behaviour. Wiley: New York.

Mowrer, 0. H. and Lamoreaux, R. R. (1946). Fear as an intervening variable in avoidance conditioning. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 39, 29—50.

Muenzinger, K. F. (1934). Motivation in learning: I. Electric shock for correct response in the visual discrimination habit. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 17, 267—77. Muncer, S. J. (1982). ‘Conservations’ with a chimpanzee, Developmental Psychobiology, 16, 1—Il.

Muncer, S. J. (1982). ‘Conversations’ with a chimpanzee. Developmental Psychobiology, 16, 1—11.

Muncer, S. J. and Ettlinger, G. (1981). Communication by a chimpanzee: first trial mastery of word order that is critical for meaning, but failure to negate conjunctions. Neuropsyhcologia, 19, 73—8.

Munn, N. L. (1950). Handbook of Psychological Research on the Rat. Houghton Mifflin: Boston.

Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive Psychology. Appleton- Century-Crofts: New York.

Neuringer, A. J. (1969). Animals respond for food in the presesnce of free food. Science, 166, 399—401.

Nicholson, J. N. and Gray, J. A. (1972). Peak shift, behavioural contrast and stimulus generalization as related to personality and development in children. British Journal of Psychology, 63, 47-62.

Nicolai J. (1959). Familientradition in der Gesangesentwicklung des Gimpels (Pyrrhula pyrhula L.. Journal fir Ornithologie, 100, 39—46.

Nissen, H. W. (1930). A study of maternal behaviour in the white rat by means of the obstruction method. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 37, 377—93.

Norman, R. J., Buchwald, J. S. and Villablanca, J. R. (1977). Classical conditioning with auditory discrimination of the eyeblink in decerebrate cats. Science, 96, 551—3.

Norton-Griffiths, M. (1967). Some ecological aspects of the feeding behaviour of the oystercatcher on the edible mussel. Ibis, 109, 4 12—24.

Norton-Griffiths, M. (1969). The organization, control and development of parental feeding in the oyster catcher. Behaviour, 34, 55—114.

Oakley, D. A. (1979a). Instrumental reversal learning and subsequent fixed ratio performance on simple and go/no-go schedules in neodecorticate rabbits. Physiological Psychology, 7, 29—42.

Oakley, D. A. (1979b). Cerebral cortex and adaptive behaviour. In Oakley, D. A. and Plotkin, H. (eds). Brain, Behaviour and Evolution. Methuen: London, 154—88.

Oakley, D. A. (1983). The varieties of memory: a phylogenetic approach. In Mayes, A. (ed.). Memory in Animals and Humans. Van Nostrand: Wokingham, 20—82.

Oakley, D. A. (1985). Cognition and imagery in animals. In Oakley, D. A. (ed). Brain and Mind. Methuen: London, 99—131.

Oakley, D. A. and Russell, I. 8. (1976). Subcortical nature of Pavlovian differentiation in the rabbit. Physiology and Behaviour, 17, 947—54.

Ohman, A., Ericksson, B. and Loftberg, I. (1975a). Phobias and preparedness: phobic versus neutral pictures as conditioned stimuli for human autonomic responses. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 4 1—5.

Ohman, A., Ericksson, G. and Olofsson, C. (l975b). One trial learning and superior resistance to extinction of autonomic responses conditioned to potentially phobic stimuli. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 619—27.

O’Keefe, J. (1979). A review of hippocampal place cells. Progress in Neurobiology, 13, 419-39.

O’Keefe, J. and Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Clarendon Press: Oxford.

Olds, J. (1958). Self-stimulation of the brain. Science, 127, 3 15-24. Olds, J. (1961). Differential effects of drives and drugs on self-stimulation at different brain sites. In Sherr, D. E. (ed.). Electrical Stimulation of the Brain. University of Texas Press: Austin, 350—66.

Olds, J. and Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal areas and other regions of rat brains. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47, 4 19-27.

Olds, M. E. and Olds, J. (1963). Approach avoidance analysis of the rat diencephalon. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 120, 259—95.

Olson, G. M. (1976). An information-processing analysis of visual memory and habituation in infants. In Tighe, T. J. and Leaton, R. N. (eds). Habituation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 239—77.

Olton, D. S. (1979). Mazes, maps, and memory. American Psychologist, 34, 583—96.

Olton, D. S. and Collison, C. (1979). Intramaze cues and ‘odor trails’ fail to direct choice behaviour on an elevated maze. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 7, 221—3.

Olton, D. S. and Papas, B. C. (1979). Spatial memory and hippocampal function. Neuropsychologia, 17, 669—82.

Olton, D. S. and Samuelson, R. J. (1976). Remembrance of places passed: spatial memory in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 2, 97—116.

Olton, D. S., Collison, C. and Werz, M. A. (1977). Spatial memory and radial arm maze performance in rats. Learning and Motivation, 8, 289-314.

Olton, D. S., Becker, J. T. and Handelmann, G. E. (1979). Hippocampus, space, and memory. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 2, 313—65.

Olton, D. S., Handelman, 0. E. and Walker, J. A. (1981). Spatial memory and food searching strategies. In Kamil, A. C. and Sargent, T. 0. (eds). Foraging Behaviour. Garland Press: New York, 333—54.

Osgood, C. E. (1953). Method and Theory in Experimental Psychology. Oxford University Press: New York.

Overman, W. H. and Doty, R. W. (1980). Prolonged visual memory in macaques and man. Neuroscience, 5, 1825-31.

Overmier, J. B. and Seligman, M. E. P. (1967). Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 28—33.

Overmier, J. B., Bull, J. A. and Trapold, M. A. (1971). Discriminative cue properties of different fears and their role in response selection in dogs. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 76, 478—82.

Papez,J. W. (1937). A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives ofNeurology and Psychiatry, 38, 725—43.

Park, T. J. and Dooling, R. J. (1985). Perception of species- specific contact calls by budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 99, 391—402.

Parkinson, J. K. and Medin, D. L. (1983). Emerging attributes in monkey short-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 9, 31-40.

Pasnak, R. (1979). Acquisition of prerequisites to conservation by macaques. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 5, 194-210.

Passingham, R. (1982). The Human Primate. W. H. Freeman: Oxford. Patterson, F. G. (1978). The gestures of a gorilla: language acquisition in another pongid. Brain and Language, 5, 72—99.

Patterson, M. M. (1976). Mechanisms of classical conditioning and fixation in spinal mammals. In Riesen, A. H. and Thompson, R. F. (eds). Advances in Psychobiology, Vol, 3. Wiley: New York.

Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes: An Investigation of the Physiological Activity of the Cerebral Cortex. Dover: New York.

Pavlov, I. P. (1955). Selected Works. Foreign Languages Publishing House; Moscow.

Pearce, J. M. and Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532—52.

Peeke, H. V. S. (1983). Habituation, sensitization, and redirection of aggression and feeding behaviour in the three- spined stickleback (Gasterosteurs aculeatus L.). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 97, 43—5 1.

Peeke, H. V. S. and Petrinovich, L. (1984). Habituation, Sensitization, and Behaviour. Academic Press: London.

Peele, D. B., Casey,J., and Silberger, A. (1984). Primacy of IRT reinforcement in accounting for rate differences under variable-ratio and variable-interval schedules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 10, 149—67.

Penfield, W. and Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and Brain Mechanisms. Princeton University Press.

Pepperberg, I. M. (1981). Functional vocalizations by an African grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). Zeitschrzfl fόr Tierpsycholgoie, 55, 139—60.

Pepperberg, I. M. (1983). Cognition in the African grey parrot: preliminary evidence for auditory/vocal comprehension of the class concept. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 11, 179—85.

Perkins, C. C. (1968). An analysis of the concept of reinforcement. Psychological Review, 75, 155—72.

Perrett, D. I., Smith, R. A. J., Potter, D. D., Mistlin, A. J., Head, A. S., Mimner, A. D. and ,Jeeves, M. A. (1985). Visual cells in the temporal cortex sensitive to face view and gaze direction. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 223, 293—317.

Petersen, M. R., Beecher, M. D., Zoloth, S. R., Moody, D. and Stebbins, W. L. (1978). Neural lateralization of species- specific vocalizations by Japanese Macques (Macaca fusata). Science, 202, 324-7.

Peterson, C. and Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causam explanations as a risk factor for depression: theory and evidence. Psychological Review, 91, 347—74.

Peterson, G. B. (1984). How expectancies guide behaviour. In Roitbiat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Ermbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, m 35—48.

Peterson, G. B., Wheeler, R. L. and Armstrong, G. D, (1978). Expectancies as mediators. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 6, 279—85.

Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and Knowledge. Edinburgh University Press.

Piaget, J. (1971). Biology and Knowledge. Edinburgh University Press.

Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1969). The Psychology of the Child. Routledge & Kegan Paul: London.

Pierrel, R. and Sherman, J. G. (1960). Generamization and discrimination as a function of the SD’ — SS difference. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 5, 67—7 l.

Pinel, J. P. J. and Treit, D. (m978). Burying as a defensive response in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 92, 708—12.

Pinel, J. P. J. and Treit, D. (1979). Conditioned defensive burying in rats: availabimity of burying materiams. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 7, 392—6.

Pinsker, H., Kupfermann, I., Castellucci, V. and Kandel, E. (1970). Habituation and dishabituation of the gill withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. Science, 167, 1740—2.

Plato (c. 400 BC/191 7—37). Plato with an English Translation. Heinemann: London.

Platt, J. R. (1979). Interresponse time shaping by variable- interval like reinforcement contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 31, 3—14.

Pliskoff, S. S., Wright, J. E. and Hawkins, B. T. (1965). Brain stimulation as a reinforcer: intermittent schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 8, 75—88.

Plotkin, FL C. and Odling-Smee, F. J. (1981). A multiple-level model of evolution and its implications for sociobiology. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 4, 225—268.

Podgorny, P. and Garner, W. R. (1979). Reaction time as a measure of inter— and intraobject visual simimarity. Perception and Psychophysics, 26, 37—52.

Polit, A. and Bizzi, E. (1978). Processes controlling arm movements in monkeys. Science, 201, 1235—7,

Porter, D. and Neuringer, A. (1984). Music discrimination by pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 10, 138—48.

Powell, R. W. (1979). The effect of reinforcement magnitude upon responding under fixed-ratio schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 12, 605—8.

Prelec, D. (1982). Matching, maximizing, and the hyperbolic reinforcement feedback function. Psychological Review, 89, 189—230.

Prelec, D. (1984). The assumptions underlying the generalized matching law. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 41, 10 1—7.

Premack, D. (1965). Reinforcement theory. In Levine, D. (ed.). Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. University of Nebraska Press, 123—80.

Premack, D. (1971). Language in chimpanzee. Science, 172, 808—22. Premack, D. (1976). Intelligence in Ape and Man. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey.

Premack, D. (1983). The codes of man and beasts. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 6, 125—37.

Premack, D. and Woodruff, G. (1978). Chimpanzee problem solving: a test for comprehension. Science, 202, 532—5.

Premack, D., Woodruff, G. and Kennel, K. (1978). Paper-marking test for chimpanzee: simple control for social cues. Science, 202, 903—5.

Pryor, K. W., Haag, R. and O’Reilly, J. (1969). The creative porpoise: training for novel behaviour. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 12, 653—61.

Pyke, G. H., Pulliam, H. R. and Charnov, E. L. (1977). Optimal foraging: a selective review of theory and tests. Quarterly Review of Biology, 52, 137—54.

Rachlin, H. (1978). A molar theory of reinforcement schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 30, 345—60.

Rachlin, H. and Baum, W. M. (1969). Response rate as a function of amount of reinforcement for a signalled concurrent response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 12, 11—16.

Rachlin, H., Battalio, R., Kagel, J. and Green, L. (1981). Maximization theory in behavioural psychology. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 4, 371—417.

Rachman, S. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear- acquisition a critical examination. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375—87.

Rawlins, J. P. N. (1985). Associations across time: the hippocampus as a temporary memory store. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 8, 479—96.

Reason, J. and Mycielska, K. (1982). Absent-Minded?: The Psychology of Mental Lapses and Everyday Error. Prentice- Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

Redshaw, M. (1978), Cognitive development in human and gorilla infants. Journal of Human Evolution, 7, 133—41.

Rosenzweig, S. (1943), An experimental study of ‘repression’ with special reference to need-persistive and ego-defensive reactions to frustration. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 64—74.

Ross, R. T., Orr, W. B., Holland, P. C. and Berger, T. W. (1984). Hippocampectomy disrupts acquisition and retention of learning conditional responding. Behavioural Neuroscience, 98, 211—25.

Rosselini, R. A. (1978). Inescapable shock interferes with the acquisition of a free appetitive operant. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 6, 155—9.

Rosselini, R. A. and DeCola, J. P. (1981). Inescapable shock interferes with the acquisition of a low-activity response in an appetitive context. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 9, 487—90.

Rosselini, R. A., DeCola, J. P., and Shapiro, N. R. (1982). Cross-motivational effects of inescapable shock are associative in nature. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 8, 376—88.

Rozin, P. and Kalat, J. W. (1971). Specific hungers and poison avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychological Review, 78, 459—86.

Rumbaugh, D. M, and Pate, J. L. (1984). The evolution of cognition in primates: a comparative perspective. In Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, 568—87.

Rumbaugh, D. M. and von Glaserfeld, E. (1973). Reading and sentence completion by a chimpanzee. Science, 182, 73 1—3.

Russell, B. (1940). An lnquiry into Meaning and Truth. Allen & Unwin: London.

Russell, B. (1946). The History of Western Philosophy. Allen & Unwin: London.

Russell, M., Dark, K. A., Cummins, R. W., Ellman, G., Calloway, E. and Peeke, H. V. S. (1984). Learned histamine release. Science, 225, August 17,733—4.

Sahley, C., Rudy, J. W. and Gelperin, A. (1981). An analysis of associative learning in a terrestial mollusc: I. Higher order conditioning, blocking, and a transient US pre-exposure effect. Journal of Comparative Physiology, A, 144, 1—8.

Sands, S. F. and Wright, A. A. (l980a). Primate memory: retention of serial list items by a rhesus monkey. Science, 209, 938—40.

Sands, S. F. and Wright, A. A. (1980b). Serial probe recognition performance by a rhesus monkey and a human with 10— and 20-item lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 6, 386—96.

Sands, S. F., Urcuioli, P. J., Wright, A. A. and Santiago, H. C. (1984). Serial position effects and rehearsal in primate visual memory. In Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, 375—88.

Santiago, H. C. and Wright, A. A. (1984). Pigeon memory: same/dzfferent concept learning, serial probe recognition acquisition, and probe delay effects on the serial position function. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 10, 498—512.

Savage, G. E. (1980). The fish telencephalon and its relation to learning. In Ebbesson, S. 0. E. (ed.). Comparative Neurology of the Telencephalon. Plenum Press: New York, 129—74.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M. and Boysen, S. (1978a). Symbolic communication between two chimpanzees. Science, 201, 641—4.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Rumbaugh, D. M. and Boysen, S. (1978b). Linguistically mediated tool use and exchange by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 1, 539—54.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. Rumbaugh, D. M., Smith, S. T. and Lawson, J. (1980). Reference: the linguistic essential. Science, 210, 922—5.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Pate, J. L., Lawson, J., Smith, S. T. and Rosen-baum, S. (1983). Can a chimpanzee make a statement?. Journal of Exper-imental Psychology: General, 112, 457—92.

Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Sevik, R. A., Rumbaugh, D. M. and Rubert, E. (1985). The capacity of animals to acquire language: do species differences have anything to say to us?. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 308, 177—85.

Schachter, S. and Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, social and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379—99.

Schneider, J. W., (1973). Reinforcer effectiveness as a function of rate and magnitude: a comparison of concurrent performances. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 20, 461—71.

Schneider, W. and Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1-66.

Schrier, A., Angarella, R. and Povar, M. (1984). Studies of concept formation by stumptailed monkeys: concepts humans, monkeys, and letter A. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 10, 564—84.

Schusterman, R. J. and Krieger, K. (1984). California sea lions are capable of semantic comprehension. Psychological Record, 34, 3—23.

Seligman, M. E. P. (1968). Chronic fear produced by unpredictable electric shock. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 66, 402—11.

Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychological Review, 77, 406—18.

Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). On Depression, Development and Death. W. H. Freeman: San Francisco.

Seligman, M. E. P. and Beagley, G. (1975). Learned helplessness in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 534—41.

Seligman, M. E. P. and Hager,J. C. (1972). Biological Boundaries of Learning. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York.

Seligman, M. E. P. and Johnston, J. C. (1973). A cognitive theory of avoidance learning. In McGuigan, F. J. and Lumsden, D. B. (eds). Contemporary Approaches to Conditioning and Learning. Winston-Wiley: Washington, DC.

Seligman, M. E. P. and Weiss, J. M. (1980). Learned helplessness, physiological change, and learned inactivity. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 18, 459—73.

Seligman, M. E. P., Maier, S. F. and Geer, H. H. (1968). Alleviation of learned helplessness in the dog. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 73, 256—62.

Selye, H. (1950). The Physiology and Pathology of Exposure to Stress. Acta: Montreal.

Seward, J. P. (1949). An experimental analysis of latent learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 177- 86.

Shapiro, M. and Miller, T. M. (1965). On the relationship between conditioned and discriminative stimuli and between instrumental and consummatory responses. In Prokasy, W. F. (ed.). Classical Conditioning. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York, 269—301.

Sheffield, F. D. (1965). Relation between classical conditioning and instrumental learning. in Prokasy, W. F. (ed.). Classical Conditioning. AppletonCentury-Crofts: New York, 302—22.

Sheffield, F. D. and Roby, T. B. (1950). Reward value of a non- nutritive sweet taste. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 471—81.

Sheffield, F. D., Wulff, J. J. and Backer, R. (1951). Reward value of copulation without sex drive reduction. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 44, 3—8.

Shepp, B. E. and Schrier, A. M. (1969). Consecutive intradimensional and extradimensional shifts in monkeys. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 67, 199—203.

Sherman, J. A. and Thomas, J. R. (1968). Some factors controlling preference between fixed-ratio and variable-ratio schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 11, 689—702.

Sherrington, C. S. (1906). The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. Yale University Press: New Haven.

Shettleworth, S. J. (1983). Memory in food-hoarding birds. Scientific American, 3, (March), 86—94.

Shettleworth, S. J. and Krebs, J. R. (1982). How marsh tits find their hoards: the roles of site preference and spatial memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 8, 354—75.

Shiffrin, R. M. and Schneider, W. (1984). Automatic and controlled processing revisited. Psychological Review, 91, 269—76.

Shimp, C. P. (1969). Optimal behaviour in free-operant experiments. Psychological Review, 76, 97—112.

Shimp, C. P. (1976). Short-term memory in the pigeon: the previously reinforced response. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 26, 487—93.

Shimp, C. P. (1984). Self report by rats of the temporal patterning of their behaviour: a dissociation between tacit knowledge and knowledge. in Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, 215-29.

Shurrager, P. S. and Culler, E. A. (1940). Conditioning in the spinal dog. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 26, 133—59.

Sidman, M. (1953). Avoidance conditioning with brief shock and no exteroceptive signal. Science, 118, 157-8.

Siegel, S. (1975). Evidence from rats that morphine tolerance is a learned response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 89, 498—506.

Siegel, S. (1976). Morphine analgesic tolerance: its situation specificity supports a Pavlovian conditioning model. Science, 193, 323—5.

Siegel, S. (1977). Morphine tolerance acquisition as an associative process. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 3, 1—13.

Siegel, S., Hinson, R. E., and Krank, M. D. (1978). The role of predrug signals in morphine analgesis tolerance: support for a Pavlovian conditioning model of tolerance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 4, 188—96.

Silberberg, A. and Fantino, E. (1970). Choice, rate of reinforcement, and the change-over delay. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 13, 187—97.

Sinnamon, H. M., Freniere, S., and Kootz, J. (1978). Rat hippocampus and memory for places of changing significance. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 92, 142—55.

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behaviour of Organisms. Appleton-Century-Crofts: New York.

Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and Human Behaviour. Macmillan: New York. Skinner, B. F. (1972). Beyond Freedom and Dignity. Jonathan Cape: London.

Skinner, B. F. (1977). Herrnstein and the evolution of behaviourism. American Psychologists, 32, 1006—12.

Slotnik, B. M. and Katz, H. M. (1974). Olfactory learning-set formation in rats. Science, 185, 796—8.

Small, W. S. (1901). Experimental study of the mental processes of the rat. II. American Journal of Psychology, 12, 206—39.

Smith, P. K. (1986). Play in Animals and Humans. Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Smith, W. amd Hale, E. B. (1959). Modification of social rank in the domestic fowl. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 373—5.

Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Perception and the Conditioned Reflex. Pergamon Press: Oxford.

Sokolov, E. N. (1975). The neuronal mechanisms of the orienting reflex. In Sokolov, E. N. and Vinogradova, 0. S. (eds). Neuronal Mechanisms of the Orienting Reflex. Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 217—35.

Sokolov, E. N. and Vinogradova, 0. S. (eds). (1975). Neuronal Mechanisms of the Orienting Reflex. Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 217—35.

Solomon, R. L. (1964). Punishment. American Psychologist, 19, 239—53. Solomon, R. L. (1980). The opponent-process theory of acquired motivation. American Psychologist, 35, 691—712.

Solomon, R. L. and Corbit, J. D. (1974). An opponent process theory of motivation: I. Temporal dynamics of affect. Psychological Review, 81, 119—45.

Solomon, R. L. and Wynne, L. C. (1953). Traumatic avoidance learning: acquisition in normal dogs. Psychological Monographs, 67, no. 4. Solomon, R. L. and Wynne, L. C. (1954). Traumatic avoidance learning: the principle of anxiety conservation and partial irreversibility. Psychological Review, 61, 653—85.

Solomon, R. L., Kamin, L.J. and Wynne, L. C. (1953). Traumatic avoidance learning: the outcomes of several extinction procedures with dogs. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 48, 29 1—302.

Sorabji, R. (1972). Aristotle on Memory. Duckworth: London. Spear, N. E. and Miller, R. R. (1981). Information Processing in Animals: Memory Mechanisms. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, NewJersey.

Spence, K. W. (1936). The nature of discrimination learning in animals. Psychological Review, 43, 427—49.

Spence, K. W. (1937). The differential response in animals to stimuli varying within a single dimension. Psychological Review, 44, 430—44.

Spence, K. W. (1940). Continuous versus non-continuous interpretations of discrimination learning. Psychological Review, 47, 271-88.

Spence, K. W. (1951). Theoretical interpretation of learning. In Stevens, S. S. (ed.), Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Wiley: New York, 690—729.

Spence, K. W. (1956). Behaviour Theory and Conditioning. Yale University Press: New Haven.

Spence, K. W. and Lippit, R. (1946). An experimental test of the sign-Gestalt theory of trial-and-error learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 49 1—502.

Staddon,J. E. R. (1983). Adaptive Behaviour and Learning. Cambridge University Press.

Staddon, J. E. R. and Simmelhag, V. L. (1971). The ‘superstition’ experiment: a reexamination of its implications for the principles of adaptive behaviour. Psychological Review, 78, 3—43.

Stanton, M. E.,Thomas, G. J. and Brito, G. N. 0. (1984). Posterodorsal septal lesions impair performance on both shift and stay working memory tasks. Behavioural Neuroscience, 98, 405—15.

Starr, A. and Phillips, L. (1970). Visual and motor memory in the Mnestic Syndrome. Neuropsychologyia, 8, 75—88.

Stevens, S. S. (1951). Mathematics, measurement, and psychophysics. In Stevens, S. S. (ed.). Handbook of Experimental Psychology. Wiley: New York, 1—49.

Stevenson, J., Hutchison, R. E., Hutchison, J., Bertram, B. L. R. and Thorpe, W. H. (1970). Individual recognition by auditory cues in the Common Tern (Sterna hirundo). Nature, London, 226, 562—3.db.

Stewart, J., de Wit, H. and Eikelboom, R. (1984). Role of unconditioned and conditioned drug effects in the self- administration of opiates and stimulants. Psychological Review, 91, 251-68.

Stone, C. P. (1942). Motivation. In Moss, F. A. (ed.). Comparative Psychology. Prentice-Hall: New York, 62—97.

Sulloway, F. J. (1979). Freud, Biologist of the Mind. Basic Books: New York. Sutherland, N. S. (1960). Outlines of a theory of pattern recognition in animals and man. Proceedings of the Royal Society, B, 171, 297—3 17.

Sutherland, N. S. and Mackintosh, N. J. (1971). Mechanisms of Animal Discrimination Learning. Academic Press: London.

Taub, E. and Berman, A. J. (1968). Movement and learning in the absence of sensory feedback. In Freedman, S. J. (ed.). The Neuropsychology of Spatially Oriented Behaviour. Dorsey Press: Homewood, Ill., 173-92.

Taub, E., Bacon, R. C. and Berman, A. J. (1965). Acquisition of trace-conditioned avoidance response after deafferentation of the responding limb. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 59, 275—9.

Terlecki, L. J., Pinel, J. P. L. and Triet, D. (1979). Conditioned and unconditioned defensive burying in the rat. Learning and Motivation, 10, 337—50.

Terrace, H. S. (1963). Discrimination training with and without ‘errors’. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 6, 1—27. 283.

Terrace, H. S. (1966). Behavioural contrast and the peak shift: effects of extended discrimination training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 9, 613-7.

Terrace, H. S. (1981). Introduction: autoshaping and two- factor learning theory. In Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. and Gibbon, J. (eds), Autoshaping and Conditioning Theory, Academic Press: London, 1—18.

Terrace, H. S. (1985). Animal cognition: thinking without language. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 308, 113-28.

Terrace, H. S., Pettito, L. A., Sanders, R. J. and Bever, T. G. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence. Science, 206, 89 1—902.

Thistlethwaite, D. (1951). A critical review of latent learning and related experiments. Psychological Bulletin, 48, 97—129.

Thomas, D. R. and Switalski, R. W. (1966). Comparison of stimulus generalization following variable-ratio and variable- interval training. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, 365—76.

Thomas, D. R., Freeman, F., Svinicki, J. G., Burr, D. E. S. and Lyons, J. (1970). Effects of extradimensional training on stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Monograph, 83, no. 1, part 2.

Thomas, D. R., Miller,J. T. and Svinicki,J. G. (1971). Nonspecific transfer effects of discrimination training in the rat. Journal Of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 74, 96—101.

Thomas, G. J. and Spafford, P. S. (1984). Deficits for representational memory induced by septal and cortical lesions (singly and combined) in rats. Behavioural Neuroscience, 98, 394—404.

Thompson, R. F. and Glanzman, D. L. (1976). Neural and behavioural mechanisms of habituation and sensitization. In Tighe, T. J. and Leaton, P.. N. (eds). Habituation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 49—93.

Thompson, P.. F. and Spencer, W. A. (1966). Habituation: a model phenomenon for the study of the neuronal substrates of behaviour. Psychological Review, 73, 16—43.

Thon, B. and Pauzie, A. (1984). Differential sensitization, retention, and generalization of habituation in two response systems in the blowfly (Calliphora vomitoria). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 98, 119-30.

Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: an experimental study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplements, 2(8), 1—109.

Thorndike, E. L. (190l). The mental life of the monkeys. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplements, 3(5), 1—57.

Thorndike, E. L. (1931). Human Learning. Century: London. Thorpe, W. H. (1963). Learning and Instinct in Animals. 2nd ed. Methuen: London.

Tighe, T. J. and Leaton, P.. N. (1976). Habituation: Perspectives from Child Development, Animal Behaviour, and Neurophysiology. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey.

Timberlake, W. and Grant, D. 1. (1975). Autoshaping rats to the presentation of another rat predicting food. Science, 190, 690—2.

Tinbergen, J. M. (1981). Foraging decisions in Starlings Sturnus vulgaris L. Ardea, 69, 1—67.

Tinbergen, N. (1951). The Study of Instinct. Oxford University Press: London.

Tinbergen, N. (1953). The Herring Gull’s World. Collins: London.

Tinbergen, N. and Kuenen~ D. J. (1957). Feeding behaviour in young thrushes. In Schillar, C. H. (ed.), Instinctive Behaviour: Development of a Modern Concept. Methuen: London, 209—36.

Tinbergen, N. and Perdeck, A. C. (1950). On the stimulus situation releasing the begging response in the newly-hatched herring-gull chick (Larus argentatus Pont). Behaviour, 3, 1—39.

Tinkelpaugh, 0. L. (1928). An experimental study of representative factors in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8, 197—236.

Todorov, J. C., Hanna, E. S. and Bittencourt de Sa, M. C. N. (1984). Frequency versus magnitude of reinforcement: new data with a different procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 41, 157—67.

Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive Behaviour in Animals and Man. Century: New York.

Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. Psychological Review, 55, 189—208.

Tolman, E. C. (1959). Principles of purposive behaviourism. In Koch, S. (ed.), Psychology: A Study of a Science, Vol. 2. McGraw-Hill: New York, 92—157.

Tolman, E. C. and Honzik, C. H. (l930a). ‘Insight’ in rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 215-232.

Tolman, E. C. and Honzik, C. H. (1930b). Introduction and removal of reward, and maze learning in rats. University of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 257—75.

Tolman, E. C., Ritchie, B. F. and Kalish, D. (1946). Studies in spatial learning: I. Orientation and the short cut. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36, 13—24.

Tolman, E. C., Ritchie, B. F. and Kalish, D. (1947). Studies in spatial learning: V. Response learning versus place learning by the non-correction method. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37, 285—92.

Trapold, M. A. (1970). Are expectancies based upon different positive reinforcing events discriminably different?. Learning and Motivation, 1, 129—40.

Twitmeyer, E. B. (1902/1974). A study of the knee jerk. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 103, 1047—66.

Urcioli, P. J. (1977). Transfer of oddity from sample performance in pigeons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 27, 195—202.

Urcioli, P. J. and Zentall, T. P.. (1986). Retrospective coding in pigeons’ delayed matching-to-sample. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 12, 69—770.

Van der Wall, S. B. (1982). An experimental analysis of cache recovery in Clark’s nutcracker. Animal Behaviour, 30, 84—94.

Vaughan, Jr., W. and Greene, S. L. (1984). Pigeon visual memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 10, 256—71.

Wagner, A. P. (1976). Priming in STM: an information- processing mechanism for self-generated or retrieval-generated depression in performance. In Tight, T. J. and Leaton, P.. N. (eds). Habituation. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 95— 128.

Wagner, A. R. (1978). Expectancies and the priming of STM. In Hulse, S. H., Fowler, H. and Honik, W. K. (eds). Cognitive Processes in Animal Behaviour. Lawrence Eribaum Associates: Hilisdale, NewJersey, 177—209.

Wagner, A. R. (1979). Habituation and memory. In Dickinson, A. and Boakes, R. A. (eds). Mechanisms of Learning and Motivation. Lawrence Erlbaum: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 53—82.

Wagner, A. R. (1981). SOP: a model of automatic memory processing in animal behaviour. In Spear, N. and Miller, R. R. (eds). Information Processing in Animals. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillsdale, New Jersey, 5—47.

Walker, J. A. and Olton, D. S. (1979). Spatial memory following fimbria-fornix lesions: independent of time for stimulus processing. Physiology and Behaviour, 23, 11—15.

Walker, J. A., and Olton, D. 5. (1984). Fimbria-fornix lesions impair spatial working memory but not cognitive mapping. Behavioural Neuroscience, 98, 226—42.

Walker, S. F. (1983a). Animal Thought. Routledge & Kegan Paul: London.

Walker, S. F. (1983b). The pros and cons of having a word for it. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 6, 56—7.

Walker, S. F. (1984). Learning Theory and Behaviour Modification. Methuen: London.

Walker, S. F. (1986). The evolution and dissolution of language. In Ellis, A. W. (ed.). Progress in the Psychology of Language, Vol.3. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: London, 5—48.

Walker, S. F. and Hurwitz, H. M. B. (1970). Effects of relative reinforcer duration on concurrent response rates. Psychonomic Science, 22, 45—7.

Walker, S. F., Schnelle, J. and Hurwitz, H. M. B. (1970). Rates of concurrent responses and reinforcer duration. Psychonomic Science, 21, 173—5.

Warden, C. J. (1931). Animal Motivation Studies. The Albino Rat, Columbia University Press: New York.

Warner, L. H. (1927). A study of sex behaviour in the white rat by means of the obstruction method. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 4, No. 22.

Warner, L. H. (1928a). A study of hunger behaviour in the white rat by means of the obstruction method. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8, 273—99.

Warner, L. H. (1928b). A study of thirst behaviour in the white rat by means of the obstruction method. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 35, 178—92.

Warren, J. M. (1965). Primate learning in comparative perspective. In Schrier, A. M., Harlow, H. F. and Stollnitz, F. (eds). Behaviour of Nonhuman Primates, Vol. 1. Academic Press: New York, 249—81.

Warren, J. M. (1973) Learning in vertebrates. In Dewsbury, D. A. and Rethlingshafer, D. A. (eds). Comparative Psychology: A Modern Survey. McGraw-Hill: New York, 471—509.

Wasserman, E. A. (1981). Response evocation in autoshaping. In Locurto, C. M., Terrace H. S. and Gibbon, J. (eds). Autoshaping and Conditioning Theory. Academic Press: London, 2 1—54.

Waters, R. S. and Wilson, W. A. (1976). Speech perception by rhesus monkeys: the voicing distinction in synthesized labial and velar stop consonants. Perception and Psychophysics, 19, 285—9.

Watson, J. B. (1907). Kinesthetic and organic sensations: their role in the reactions of the white rat. Psychological Review Monographs, 8, no. 2.

Watson,J. B. (1914/1967). Behaviour: An Introduction to Comparative Psychology. HoIt, Rinehart & Winston: London.

Watson, J. B. (1931). Behaviourism. Kegan Paul, Trench & Trubner: London.

Weidman, U. (1961). The stimuli eliciting begging responses in gulls and terns. Animal Behaviour, 9, 115—16.

Weinberger, N. M., Gold, P. E. and Sternberg, B. B. (1984). Epinephrine enables Pavlovian fear conditioning under anesthesia. Science, 223, 605—7.

Weiskrantz, L. (1974). The interaction between occipital and temporal cortex in vision: an overview. In Schmitt, F. 0. and Worden, F. G. (eds). The Neurosciences: Third Study Program. MIT Press: London, 189—204.

Weiskrantz, L. (1977). Trying to bridge some neuropsychological gaps between monkey and man. British Journal of Psychology, 68, 431—45.

Weiskrantz, L. (1985). Introduction: categorization, cleverness and consciousness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B, 308, 3—19.

Weiss,J. M. (1968). Effects of coping responses on stress. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 65,251—60.

Weiss, J. M. (1971). Effects of coping behaviour in different warning signal conditions on stress pathology in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 77,1—13.

Weiss, J. M. and Glazer, H. I. (1975). Effects of acute exposure to stressors on subsequent avoidance-escape behaviour. Psychosomatic Medicine, 37, 499—521.

Weiss, S. J. (1971). Discrimination training and stimulus compounding consideration of non-reinforcement and response differentiation consequence of sD, Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 15, 387—402.

Weissman, R. G., Wasserman, E. A., Dodd, P. D. W. and Larew, M. B. (1980). Representation and retention of two-event sequences in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 6, 312—25.

Wexler, K. (1982). A principle theory for language acquisition. In Wanner, E. and Gleitman, L. R. (eds), Language Acquisition: The State of the Art. Cambridge University Press, 288-3 15.

Wilcoxon, H., Dragoin, W. and Kral, P. (1971). Illness-induced aversions in rat and quail: relative salience of visual and gustatory cues. Science, 171, 826—8.

Williams, D. R. (1981). Biconditional behaviour: conditioning without constraint. In Locurto, C. M., Terrace, H. S. and Gibbon, J. (eds). Autoshaping and Conditioning Theory. Academic Press: London, 55—99.

Williams, D. R. and Williams, H. (1969). Auto-maintenance in the pigeon: sustained pecking despite contingent non- reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 12, 511—20.

Williams, J. H. (1950). Elephant Bill. Hart-Davis: London,

Wilson, B., Mackintosh, N. J. and Boakes, R. A. (1985). Transfer of relational rules in matching and oddity learning by pigeons and corvids. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 378, 313—22.

Wilson, E. 0. (1975). Sociobiology. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, Mass.

Wise, K. L., Wise, L. A. and Zimmermann, R. R. (1974). Piagetian object permanence in the infant rhesus monkey. Developmental Psychology, 10, 429—37.

Wolpe, J. (1958). Psychotherapy by Reciprocal inhibition. Stanford Universeity Press.

Woodard, W. T., Schoel, W, M. and Bitterman, M. E. (1971) Reversal learning with singly presented stimuli in pigeons and goldfish. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 76, 460—7.

Woodruff, G., Premack, D. and Kennel, K. (1978). Conservation of liquid and solid quantity by the chimpanzee. Science, 202, 99 1-4.

Woods, S. C. (1976). Conditioned hypoglycemia. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 90, 1164—8.

Woods, S. C. and Kulkosky, P. J. (1976). Classically conditioned changes of blood glucose level. Psychosomatic Medicine, 38, 20 1—19.

Wright, A. A., Santiago, H. C. and Sands, S. F. (1983). On the nature of the primacy effect in memory processing: a reply to Gaffan. Animal Learning and Behaviour, 11, 148—50.

Wright, A. A., Santiago, H. C. and Sands, S. F. (1984a). Monkey memory: same/djfferent concept learning, serial probe recognition acquisition, and probe delay effects on the serial position function. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 10, 513—29.

Wright, A. A., Santiago, H. C., Sands, S. F. and Urcioli, P. J. (1984b). Pigeon and monkey serial probe recognition: acquisition strategies, and serial position effects. In Roitblat, H. L., Bever, T. G. and Terrace, H. S. (eds). Animal Cognition. Lawrence Eribaum Associates: London, 353—73.

Wright, D. L., French, G. M. and Riley, D. A. (1968). Similarity responding by monkeys in a matching-to-sample task. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 65,191—6.

Yamaguchi, H. (1961). The effect of continuous, partial, and varied magnitude reinforcement on acquisition and extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 319—21.

Yarczower, M. (1971). Stimulus control during conditional discrimination. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 16, 89—94.

Yates, A. (1980). Biofeedback and the Mod ification of Behaviour. Plenum Press: New York.

Yates, F. A. (1966). The Art of Memory. Routledge & Kegan Paul: London.

Yeo, C. H. (1979). The anatomy of the vertebrate nervous system: an evolutionary and developmental perspective. In Oakley, D. A. and Plotkin, H. (eds). Brain Behaviour and Evolution. Methuen: London, 28-51.

Zeiler, M. D. (1971) Eliminating behaviour with reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 16,400—3.

Zentall, D. B. Hogan, D. E., Edwards, C. A. and Hearst, E. (1980). Oddity learning in the pigeon as a function of the number of incorrect alternatives. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behaviour Processes, 6, 278—89.

Ziriax, J. M. and Silberberg, A. (1984). Concurrent variable- interval variable-ratio schedules can provide only weak evidence for matching. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 41, 83—100.

Zurif, G. E. (1970). A comparison of variable-ratio and variable-interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behaviour, 13, 369—74.